Variation in the shape of speech organs influences language evolution

Variation in the shape of speech organs influences language evolution
The anatomy of the vocal tract shows continuous and overlapping but identifiable variation between broad ethno-linguistic groups. We show the results of the Canonical Variate Analysis (CVA) of 57 classical anthropological measurements of the oral vocal tract derived from the 3D intra-oral optical scans of n=94 participants from the ArtiVarK sample [ref16], distributed in four broad self-declared ethno-linguistic groups. These groups are: "Ca" = European or North American of European Descent, speaking Indo-European (mostly Germanic and Romance) languages; "NI" = North Indian, speaking Indo-Aryan languages; "SI" = South Indian, speaking Dravidian languages; and "C" = Chinese, speaking Sino-Tibetan languages. Panels (a) and (b) show the distribution of the participants (represented by their group) in the space of the first three Canonical Axes (CVs; explaining, sequentially, 49.3%, 37.6% and 13.1% of variance); the solid polygons are the convex hulls and the colored ellipses are the 95% confidence ellipses. Panel (c) plots the posterior probabilities of each participant belonging to the four groups (vertical bars), while the top symbols show the actual group (the squares) and the assigned group (the circles; gray circles represent "outlier" participants which cannot be assigned to any group because they are below the horizontal solid line of the 5% threshold); the dotted horizontal line shows the probability of 1.0. In this case, CVA is very successful at recovering the groups despite a few misclassifications and "outliers" (84% overall classification accuracy) and it can be seen that, while overlapping, the four groups are separated by the first three CVs. Credit: Dan Dediu / Scott Moisik

Why do languages sound so different when people across the world have roughly the same speech organs (mouth, lips, tongue and jaw)? Does the shape of our vocal tract explain some of the variation in speech sounds? In extreme individual cases, it clearly does: When children are born with a cleft palate, the roof of the mouth is not formed properly, which affects their speech. However, it is unclear whether subtle anatomical differences between normal speakers play a role.

Language and speech are also shaped by repeated use and transmission from parents to children. As language is passed on to new generations, small differences may sometimes be amplified. This observation led a team based at the Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics in Nijmegen, the Netherlands, to ask what happens when tiny differences in vocal tract anatomy meet cultural transmission.

The team decided to focus on whether the of the hard palate might influence the way vowels are learned, articulated and passed on across generations of artificial agents. Because changing the shape of the hard palate in human participants is ethically and practically problematic, the scientists opted for a computational study, adapting an existing computer model of the vocal tract.

The team imported actual hard palate shapes from more than 100 MRI scans of human participants into the computer model. Via machine learning, they trained agents to articulate five common vowels, such as the 'ee' sound in "beet" and the 'oo' sound in "boot." Next, a second tried to learn these particular vowels, which were then passed on to the next generation, and so on for 50 generations.

"This simulates a simple model of language change and evolution in a computer," explains co-author Rick Janssen, currently specialist at ALTEN and Philips Research in the Netherlands. Would subtle anatomical differences in palate shape lead to differences in pronunciation? And crucially, would these differences become more pronounced through repeated transmission?

Biology matters

The subtle differences in the shape of the hard palate did influence how accurately the five vowels were articulated. Importantly, the cultural transmission of across generations amplified these small differences, even though the agents actively tried to compensate for their hard palate shape by using other articulators (such as the tongue). "Even small variations in the shape of our vocal tract may affect the way we speak, and this may even be amplified—across generations—to the level of differences between dialects and languages. Thus, biology matters," explains the lead author, Dan Dediu, currently at the Laboratoire Dynamique Du Langage, Université Lumière Lyon 2 in France.

According to the authors, this result may also help researchers to better understand the effects of anatomical variation on speech and how to correct it when desired, for instance, in case of speech pathology, forensic linguistics, dentistry and post-surgery recovery. But most importantly, the study highlights the importance of individual variation in and in the context of our universal similarities: Co-author Scott Moisik, currently at the School of Humanities, Nanyang Technological University in Singapore, concludes: "While we are all humans and fundamentally the same, we are also unique individuals, and one can really hear it."

The study is published in Nature Human Behaviour.

More information: Weak biases emerging from vocal tract anatomy shape the repeated transmission of vowels, Nature Human Behaviour (2019). DOI: 10.1038/s41562-019-0663-x , www.nature.com/articles/s41562-019-0663-x

Journal information: Nature Human Behaviour

Provided by Max Planck Society

Citation: Variation in the shape of speech organs influences language evolution (2019, August 19) retrieved 19 March 2024 from https://phys.org/news/2019-08-variation-speech-language-evolution.html
This document is subject to copyright. Apart from any fair dealing for the purpose of private study or research, no part may be reproduced without the written permission. The content is provided for information purposes only.

Explore further

Baboon vocalizations contain five vowel-like sounds comparable to those of human speech

259 shares

Feedback to editors