Researchers outline how humans reduce uncertainty in social situations

people
Credit: CC0 Public Domain

Do my friends find me funny? Am I making a good impression during this job interview? For most, such questions and concerns are a routine part of life.

A new perspective paper from Brown University scientists establishes a framework to apply rigorous mathematical models of originally developed for non-, such as whether or not to purchase a lottery ticket, to such as determining an interviewer's opinion of an interviewee.

"Humans are predicting machines—our whole lives are spent trying to figure out what is the best move to do next," said Oriel FeldmanHall, an assistant professor of cognitive, linguistic and psychological sciences at Brown. "In general, another person's motivations, desires or beliefs are hidden, so we have to figure out how to navigate through the world when we're interacting with other people without that knowledge. What our next action is going to be depends on how others respond."

The framework was a collaboration between FeldmanHall and Amitai Shenhav, also a Brown assistant professor of cognitive, linguistic and psychological sciences. In the paper, they explore the sources of social uncertainty and how people react to social uncertainty, and they propose a model of three interrelated methods people use to reduce social uncertainty.

The paper was published on Monday, April 22, in the journal Nature Human Behavior.

The three ways people reduce social uncertainty range from automatic, almost instinctive processes to more cognitively demanding processes.

The authors define the first method, automatic inference, as the process of predicting another person's behavior based on their appearance and the social norms of the environment. The second method, controlled inference, is the process of updating initial impressions using new information such as putting yourself in the other person's shoes and imaging how you would behave in that situation. Social learning, the third method, involves updating your beliefs and actions using past experiences or secondhand information of the person's past behavior. People use all three processes to different extents in order to reduce their social uncertainty.

One example presented in the paper to illustrate the three ways people resolve social uncertainty involves lending a stranger money. For automatic inference, one person might be willing to lend more money to another who appears trustworthy or kind. However, deciding to lend more money to someone based on sharing an emotional connection or evidence of shared values is an example of controlled inference. Social learning could then come into play if the lender hears from a friend that the recipient is trustworthy.

The authors also discuss the negative consequences of mental shortcuts people use to reduce social uncertainty.

"First impressions and stereotypes serve a purpose as we live in very noisy worlds where we have to make snap judgements about people all the time," said FeldmanHall, who with Shenhav is affiliated with Brown's Carney Institute for Brain Science. "You can't get a holistic picture of every single person you encounter on the street, but the reliance on first impressions can go wrong when people are unwilling to seek more information and get a bigger picture of who the person is, and instead stay narrowly focused on their first impression."

FeldmanHall is continuing her research on social uncertainty while incorporating the paper's quantitative framework to model a study on how people learn to trust one another in recurrent money-lending scenarios. She will also study how individuals understand their social networks by tracking incoming college students as they form their social networks and as these networks evolve over several years.

Shenhav will continue his research modeling the neural pathways people use to make decisions, or resolve uncertainty in non-social situations, and how they motivate themselves to overcome cognitively demanding tasks. In the future, Shenhav would like to see how motivation and effort develop in children who experience early life stress.

More information: Resolving uncertainty in a social world, Nature Human Behavior (2019). DOI: 10.1038/s41562-019-0590-x , www.nature.com/articles/s41562-019-0590-x

Journal information: Nature Human Behaviour

Provided by Brown University

Citation: Researchers outline how humans reduce uncertainty in social situations (2019, April 22) retrieved 18 April 2024 from https://phys.org/news/2019-04-outline-humans-uncertainty-social-situations.html
This document is subject to copyright. Apart from any fair dealing for the purpose of private study or research, no part may be reproduced without the written permission. The content is provided for information purposes only.

Explore further

People more likely to trust, cooperate if they can tolerate ambiguity, study finds

96 shares

Feedback to editors