Electro-sensing ability of African elephantnose fish shows acuteness of vision

Fish recognize their prey by electric colors
Elephantnose fish searching for food. Credit: © Maik Dobiey/Uni Bonn

The African elephantnose fish generates weak electrical pulses to navigate its environment. This localization sense bears an astonishing similarity to vision, as a study by the University of Bonn now shows. The study demonstrates that different objects have different electrical "colors." Fish use these colors to distinguish such things as their favorite food, mosquito larvae, from other small animals or plants. The study is published in the renowned journal Current Biology.

Elephantnose fish are nocturnal, which means they cannot rely on their eyes when hunting for prey. But they don't need to—they carry a kind of "electric flashlight" in their tail, which they use to generate short electrical pulses up to 80 times per second. Their skin, especially their trunk-like chin, is covered with electroreceptors, small sensors with which they can measure how these pulses are reflected by the environment.

With this electro-sense they can estimate distances, distinguish forms and materials, and differentiate between dead and living objects. Within fractions of a second, they can recognize whether mosquito larvae, their favorite food, are hiding in the gravel or sand at the bottom of their habitat. They can do this with considerable accuracy, largely ignoring the larvae of other insects.

How they do this was uncertain for a long time. Objects change the intensity of the electrical signal in a characteristic way—some reduce it significantly, others reflect it better. "However, this is not enough to clearly identify prey animals," explains Martin Gottwald of the Institute of Zoology at the University of Bonn. "For example, the signal strength also decreases as the distance increases." But there is another characteristic of living organisms: They also modify the shape of the electric pulses. But even this signal change depends on distance, size and position.

Fish recognize their prey by electric colors
The striking 'trunk' of Gnathonemus petersii is actually its chin. It has numerous electroreceptors. Credit: © Maik Dobiey/Uni Bonn

The combination of the two signal characteristics could solve these problems. The human eye works in a similar fashion. Its retina contains receptors for red, green and blue light. The brain then uses the mixing ratio to calculate the of the object we see. And this remains largely constant, no matter how large or far away the object in question is.

However, until now, there was no proof that a similar process occurs in . Nevertheless, it is clear that the animals have two different types of electric receptors. One only measures the intensity of the signal, the other additionally measures its shape. "We have now been able to demonstrate that the fish uses the relation between these two measurements to identify their prey," explains Prof. Dr. Gerhard von der Emde, who led the study.

At first, the scientists determined how intensity and shape of the localization signal behave in relation to each other depending on the type of object. "We found that this ratio is always constant for the same objects," says von der Emde. "And this applies regardless of their distance or other environmental parameters." "A mosquito larva therefore actually has a constant 'electrical color,' which is clearly different from that of other larvae, plant parts, members of the same species or other fish," adds Gottwald.

Fish recognize their prey by electric colors
The elephantnose fish produces brief electric pulses which it uses to perceive its environment. Different objects have different 'electrical colors'. In this artistic illustration, aquatic plants are for instance shown red, fish blue, members of the same species and other weakly electric fish yellow and insect larvae green. The mosquito larvae in the soil (orange) -- the favorite food of the elephantnose fish -- stand out from the background and other insect larvae due to their individual electric color. Credit: © Martin Gottwald/Uni Bonn

The researchers now examined the extent to which their laboratory animals used this information. They presented them with various electronic "mini chips" with a diameter of only one millimeter. Some chips produced different electrical colors; for example, they glowed like a mosquito larva or like other insect larvae. Other chips were electrically 'colorless,' similar to a pebble.

Hungry for chips

The effect was astonishing: If the chips were colored like their favorite food, the elephantnose fish chomped down reflexively. They let themselves be fooled in this way in 70 percent of all cases, even though the fake meals did not smell at all like typical prey. Even after numerous experiments, the animals did not learn to avoid the chips. They largely spurned differently colored chips, and even completely ignored electrically colorless ones. "This may suggest that the prey color is hardwired in the brains of the ," speculates von der Emde.

That would make sense. The electrical properties of living beings (and thus also their color) are decisively determined by their inner structure. And this cannot be changed easily. It is therefore barely feasible for a mosquito larva to simply add camouflage.

More information: Martin Gottwald et al, Electric-Color Sensing in Weakly Electric Fish Suggests Color Perception as a Sensory Concept beyond Vision, Current Biology (2018). DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2018.09.036

Journal information: Current Biology

Provided by University of Bonn

Citation: Electro-sensing ability of African elephantnose fish shows acuteness of vision (2018, November 14) retrieved 29 March 2024 from https://phys.org/news/2018-11-electro-sensing-ability-african-elephantnose-fish.html
This document is subject to copyright. Apart from any fair dealing for the purpose of private study or research, no part may be reproduced without the written permission. The content is provided for information purposes only.

Explore further

Elephantnose fish 'see' with their chin

18 shares

Feedback to editors