Ants follow Fermat's principle of least time

Ants follow Fermat's principle of least time
A ‘refracted’ trail of Wasmannia auropunctata workers at the medium border between smooth (white) and rough (green) felt. The position of the food is on the rough felt. The density of workers on the rough felt is higher than on the smooth felt because travel speed is lower. In addition, it appears, although not very obvious, as if the ants on the rough felt ‘float’ on top of the felt hairs, indicating the difficulty of walking on this substrate. Credit: Simon Tragust.

(Phys.org) —Ants have long been known to choose the shortest of several routes to a food source, but what happens when the shortest route is not the fastest? This situation can occur, for example, when ants are forced to travel on two different surfaces, where they can walk faster on one surface than on the other. In a new study, scientists have found that ants behave the same way as light does when traveling through different media: both paths obey Fermat's principle of least time, taking the fastest route rather than the most direct one. Besides revealing insight into ant communities, the findings could offer inspiration to researchers working on solving complex problems in robotics, logistics, and information technology.

The researchers, Jan Oettler, et al., from institutions in Germany, France, and China, have published their paper on using Fermat's principle of least time to predict ant trails in a recent issue of PLOS ONE.

The scientists experimentally studied the behavior of the little , Wasmannia auropunctata, one of the world's 100 most . They collected three colonies, each containing several thousand workers and multiple queens, from different sites in Israel. They put the in plastic boxes without food, and then connected each box to its own foraging arena where were provided as food in the corner opposite from the entrance.

The surface of each foraging arena was split in half, and each half was covered by a different material. The researchers experimented with combinations of three materials that differentially affected the ants' average walking speed: rough polyester felt (1.73 mm/s), smooth polyester felt (2.97 mm/s), and glass (4.89 mm/s). When ants , they deposit pheromones to build a trail, and favorable trails become reinforced with increased usage.

Given the ants' average walking speeds on different materials, along with information on the distances across the foraging arena and the angle defining the shortest path (the straight line) between the entrance and the food, the researchers predicted the angle at which the ants would cross from one surface to the other if they chose the fastest path. In accordance with Fermat's principle of least time, this angle can be thought of as the angle of refraction, similar to how light refracts at a certain angle at the boundary of two media in which it travels at different speeds.

Ants follow Fermat's principle of least time
The researchers calculated the fastest path as predicted by Fermat’s principle of least time (dotted yellow line), which is not the direct path. In this example, the ants can walk faster on Surface 1 than on Surface 2. Credit: Jan Oettler, et al.

The researchers found that the ants' paths closely matched those predicted by Fermat's principle of least time and did not follow the most direct route. In other words, the ants accounted for the different walking speeds on the different surfaces and traveled a longer distance on the surface where they could walk faster.

"We found that a general rule applies to a dynamic system that relies solely on communication (pheromones) and social cooperation," Oettler told Phys.org. "This system depends on two features. One is routing information that decays over time and needs to be refreshed, thus making the system flexible. And second on behavioral flexibility by the worker ants that carry this information. A path can only be adjusted by worker ants that do not follow this path, but rather take alternative routes (that they advertise), which may be faster (or slower) than the already established path."

Oettler added that the speed with which information decays is crucial to this system, so the results here may be adapted to information routing problems.

The researchers noted that Wasmannia also resemble humans in this way, since humans have also been found to follow Fermat's principle when forced to cross surfaces with different qualities. One clear example is when a lifeguard has to find the fastest way to traverse both beach and water in order to reach a drowning swimmer.

The researchers also noted that several other factors likely influence why ants choose the paths they do. For instance, the results here suggest that ants prefer to move along edges of borders (in real life, for example, ants are often seen walking along the edges of a sidewalk). Landmarks facilitate orientation, and for ants this could mean that lower amounts of pheromone are needed to mark trails when combined with the physical cues. This is one area that the researchers hope to study more in the future.

"We have only shown the outcome of this process," Oettler said. "In the future we want to study the dynamics of the trail pheromones, their active compounds and evaporation rates. We want to perform behavioral tests with synthetic compounds and detect perception thresholds of individual ants. We also want to study the early dynamics of trail formation over time."

More information: Jan Oettler, et al. "Fermat's Principle of Least Time Predicts Refraction of Ant Trails at Substrate Borders." PLOS ONE. DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0059739.g001 http://www.plosone.org/article/info%3Adoi%2F10.1371%2Fjournal.pone.0059739

Journal information: PLoS ONE

Copyright 2013 Phys.org
All rights reserved. This material may not be published, broadcast, rewritten or redistributed in whole or part without the express written permission of Phys.org.

Citation: Ants follow Fermat's principle of least time (2013, April 1) retrieved 23 April 2024 from https://phys.org/news/2013-04-ants-fermat-principle.html
This document is subject to copyright. Apart from any fair dealing for the purpose of private study or research, no part may be reproduced without the written permission. The content is provided for information purposes only.

Explore further

Robot ants successfully mimic real colony behavior

2 shares

Feedback to editors